Reward devaluation
This article, Reward devaluation, has recently been created via the Articles for creation process. Please check to see if the reviewer has accidentally left this template after accepting the draft and take appropriate action as necessary.
Reviewer tools: Inform author |
This article, Reward devaluation, has recently been created via the Articles for creation process. Please check to see if the reviewer has accidentally left this template after accepting the draft and take appropriate action as necessary.
Reviewer tools: Inform author |
Reward devaluation refers to a psychological and neurobiological phenomenon where the subjective value or motivational significance of a reward diminishes over time, often due to repeated exposure, satiation, or changes in contextual relevance[1].
Overview
This process is critical in behavioral neuroscience, cognitive psychology, and economics, as it influences decision-making, learning, and adaptive behavior. For example, animals or humans may reduce their effort to obtain a reward if its perceived value decreases (e.g., due to overconsumption or negative associations). The concept is often studied in paradigms like operant conditioning, where devaluation of a reinforcer (e.g., food) can weaken previously learned behaviors. Clinically, aberrant reward devaluation mechanisms are implicated in psychiatric conditions such as depression , where patients exhibit anhedonia (diminished pleasure in rewarding activities) and a hypersensitivity to negative outcomes , potentially reflecting dysregulated interactions between the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC)[2], anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)[3], and basolateral amygdala (BLA)[4]. These regions are thought to underlie the inability to update reward values in response to changing environmental or internal states, perpetuating motivational deficits. The phenomenon also plays a role in understanding disorders like addiction, where impaired devaluation mechanisms may drive compulsive reward-seeking despite adverse consequences.
References
- ^ Rolls, Barbara J. (27 April 2009). "Sensory-specific Satiety". Nutrition Reviews. 44 (3): 93–101. doi:10.1111/j.1753-4887.1986.tb07593.x. PMID 3515243.
- ^ Pickens, Charles L.; Saddoris, Michael P.; Gallagher, Michela; Holland, Peter C. (2005). "Orbitofrontal Lesions Impair Use of Cue-Outcome Associations in a Devaluation Task". Behavioral Neuroscience. 119 (1): 317–322. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.119.1.317. PMC 1201523. PMID 15727536.
- ^ Yuan, Zhengwei; Qi, Zhongyang; Wang, Ruiyu; Cui, Yuting; An, Sile; Wu, Guoli; Feng, Qiru; Lin, Rui; Dai, Ruicheng; Li, Anan; Gong, Hui; Luo, Qingming; Fu, Ling; Luo, Minmin (December 2023). "A corticoamygdalar pathway controls reward devaluation and depression using dynamic inhibition code". Neuron. 111 (23): 3837–3853.e5. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2023.08.022.
- ^ Wellman, Laurie L.; Gale, Karen; Malkova, Ludise (4 May 2005). "GABA A -Mediated Inhibition of Basolateral Amygdala Blocks Reward Devaluation in Macaques". The Journal of Neuroscience. 25 (18): 4577–4586. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2257-04.2005. PMC 6725040. PMID 15872105.